Once connected to the device, the children were asked to take part in a visual test which involved blinking displays of coloured squares. Visual working memory is an easy marker for early cognitive development and allows children with no reading or writing ability to be tested and compared.
The activity measured how well the children could remember the colours of the squares and if they had noticed that one side of the square always changed colour while the other stayed the same throughout the test.
After the test, other information about the child including parental education, religion, number of children in the family and economic status and income was also collected.
The results showed children from low income families showed weaker brain activity and a higher susceptibility to being distracted. Through the blood flow data they were able to trace this back to poorer distractor suppression in the left frontal cortex which is the part of the brain involved in working memory.
The children who performed the worst on the test not only came from low income homes but also had mothers with low levels of formal education.
The research, published in the journal Developmental Science, suggests that increasing household income is only one factor when it comes to improving outcomes for children growing up in poverty.
Based on this, and knowledge around brain plasticity in children, more resources with measurable outcomes need to be spent on early childhood education programmes that are designed to boost and strengthen brain health for our most at-risk children.
Mentoring programmes with adults with formal education qualifications could also help to socialise at-risk children with positive role models to help to break the cycle of inter-generational poverty through poor educational achievement.
This Government is committed to reducing child poverty and improving the wellbeing of all children and young people. Perhaps it's time to look at new research for how we might use scientific evidence to do this.