But now try a harder question: If you fold a piece of paper on itself 25 times, will the result be taller than the Empire State Building?
Most people would say no, even though the actual thickness would be about 10 miles. Asking a larger group to vote would only increase the certainty of getting it wrong.
The Max Planck researchers show that smaller groups perform particularly well when questions come in an unpredictable mix of easy and difficult. Under general conditions, they suggest, the optimal group size for making good decisions is fairly small -- often around 10 to 15, and typically less than 40.
No wonder decision-making bodies around the world work with small numbers.
Think of juries, parish councils, central bank boards or parliamentary committees, which tend to have between five and 40 members.
Granted, this research might not apply directly to the UK referendum, which arguably didn't have a right answer.
Voters clearly expressed their discontent on a number of issues, including immigration and globalization.
Yet it certainly suggests that a referendum was an awfully crude instrument for deciding such an important and difficult issue -- especially given that the British public holds wildly distorted views on, say, the number of immigrants in the country (estimated by the public at more than twice the actual level).
Voters clearly expressed their discontent on a number of issues, including immigration and globalization. UK leaders can't ignore this, but they should also question the naïve view that respecting democracy demands invoking Article 50 of the Lisbon Treaty, triggering the formal process of taking the UK out of the EU.
There's a good reason that voters gave them the power and resources to examine such choices carefully. In deciding how to respect the voters' will, and whether this requires Britain to leave or stay, that is precisely what they should do.