The Listener
  • The Listener home
  • The Listener E-edition
  • Opinion
  • Politics
  • Health & nutrition
  • Arts & Culture
  • New Zealand
  • World
  • Consumer tech & enterprise
  • Food & drink

Subscriptions

  • Herald Premium
  • Viva Premium
  • The Listener
  • BusinessDesk

Sections

  • Politics
  • Opinion
  • New Zealand
  • World
  • Health & nutrition
  • Consumer tech & enterprise
  • Art & culture
  • Food & drink
  • Entertainment
  • Books
  • Life

More

  • The Listener E-edition
  • The Listener on Facebook
  • The Listener on Instagram
  • The Listener on X

NZME Network

  • Advertise with NZME
  • OneRoof
  • Driven Car Guide
  • BusinessDesk
  • Newstalk ZB
  • Sunlive
  • ZM
  • The Hits
  • Coast
  • Radio Hauraki
  • The Alternative Commentary Collective
  • Gold
  • Flava
  • iHeart Radio
  • Hokonui
  • Radio Wanaka
  • iHeartCountry New Zealand
  • Restaurant Hub
  • NZME Events

SubscribeSign In
Advertisement
Advertise with NZME.
Home / The Listener / Life

Kiwi scientist tries to crack jumping spiders’ cognitive code

By Veronika Meduna
New Zealand Listener·
14 May, 2024 04:00 AM4 mins to read

Subscribe to listen

Access to Herald Premium articles require a Premium subscription. Subscribe now to listen.
Already a subscriber?  Sign in here

Listening to articles is free for open-access content—explore other articles or learn more about text-to-speech.
‌
Save

    Share this article

    Reminder, this is a Premium article and requires a subscription to read.

The Portia jumping spider. Photo / Ximena Nelson

The Portia jumping spider. Photo / Ximena Nelson

Imagine a hunting animal whose visual acuity is similar to a cat’s, even though it’s more than 100 times smaller and equipped with a brain with only half the neurons of a cockroach. If you watch it stalk its prey, it’s cat-like too – crouching and approaching slowly before pouncing from a distance, jumping about five times its own body length.

The main difference is that this animal is doing it on more legs, says Ximena Nelson, a biologist at Canterbury University.

Yes, it’s a jumping spider. One of the jumping spider’s other endearing behaviours is that if you look at it, it will turn its head and look straight back at you.

But it is the spider’s hunting behaviour that is of most interest to biologists studying cognition. As it moves around its prey, it sometimes even loses sight of it before attacking. This suggests jumping spiders have some sort of memory or spatial ability to know where their prey is in relation to them and that they assess their options, comparing the rewards and risks, and form an attack plan.

The question Nelson wants to answer is whether jumping spiders are able to plan ahead like much larger-brained mammals. She recently became one of very few New Zealanders to receive funding from the international Human Frontier Science Program to investigate the predatory behaviour of Portia jumping spiders, which live in tropical forests and feed on other spiders, and whether good vision or their habitat determines their planning.

Unlike insects, spiders don’t have compound eyes. Of all invertebrate species, it’s only cephalopods (octopuses, squid) and jumping spiders that have camera eyes similar to ours.

Excellent vision has allowed jumping spiders to forgo web spinning. “They’ve essentially emancipated themselves from webs because their vision enables them to exploit their habitat much more fully than a web does,” says Nelson.

One step ahead: Ximena Nelson. Photo / Supplied
One step ahead: Ximena Nelson. Photo / Supplied

Portia spiders are an exception but it’s only the female that makes a temporary “scrappy web” to lay her eggs.

Advertisement
Advertise with NZME.

The first challenge for Nelson will be to design an experimental route for the spiders that restricts it from backtracking on bad decisions. In experiments, she’s already built mazes to test their forward thinking – but the spider outsmarted her by taking shortcuts.

We already know from bees and ants that social insects have evolved phenomenal behaviours, including the waggle dance bees use to communicate the location of a good food source to their hive mates. But such behaviours are optimised by the social collective, whereas “jumping spiders are famously antisocial”, Nelson says.

Discover more

Latest research on “nature vs nurture” - do we really have control over our lives?

11 Mar 04:00 PM

Little at large: Feeling hot, hot, hot - what’s going on with the sun?

09 May 12:30 AM

Dogs are man’s best friend but what do they really think of us?

22 Jan 04:00 PM

Down to the wire: How zoos are adapting to the age of extinction

03 Oct 05:00 PM

Both bees and ants are also really good at spatial navigation, finding their way back home from long distances. But for jumping spiders, it’s more difficult to determine what clues they use.

“This research is about trying to understand the factors that seem to select for, or lead to, jumping spiders having this ability to plan ahead,” she says.

Jumping spiders live just about everywhere, from 7000m up Mt Everest to beneath sand dunes. One of Nelson’s hypotheses is that their habitat needs to be of medium complexity, allowing them to both see across long distances and hide. In these habitats, planning may be possible, not only in mammals and birds with large brains, but also in small animals with tiny brains, she says.

“The whole point of doing this with Portia spiders is because here we’re talking about an animal that’s got a small fraction of the neurons of a mammal and yet it’s displaying mammal-like behaviour. It seems to be doing all of this computational stuff with far less computing elements.”

The findings may be significant because they could lead to the development of machine learning algorithms for artificial planning systems in machines with power constraints, such as rovers used in space missions, so they can do more with less.

Save

    Share this article

    Reminder, this is a Premium article and requires a subscription to read.

Advertisement
Advertise with NZME.
Advertisement
Advertise with NZME.

Latest from The Listener

LISTENER
Reliving the Rocky Horror Show: A tribute to 50 years of the cult classic

Reliving the Rocky Horror Show: A tribute to 50 years of the cult classic

27 Jun 06:05 PM

Richard O' Brien's son Linus on his remarkable 50th anniversary Rocky Horror documentary.

LISTENER
From heartache to hope: How chronic illness inspired Debbie Harwood’s comeback

From heartache to hope: How chronic illness inspired Debbie Harwood’s comeback

02 Jul 06:02 PM
LISTENER
Book of the day: Your Friend and Mine by Jessica Dettmann

Book of the day: Your Friend and Mine by Jessica Dettmann

02 Jul 06:00 PM
LISTENER
Should you use ad blockers when you browse the internet?

Should you use ad blockers when you browse the internet?

02 Jul 06:00 PM
LISTENER
Merchant Ivory: The love story behind the costume drama moguls

Merchant Ivory: The love story behind the costume drama moguls

02 Jul 06:00 PM
NZ Herald
  • About NZ Herald
  • Meet the journalists
  • Contact NZ Herald
  • Help & support
  • House rules
  • Privacy Policy
  • Terms of use
  • Competition terms & conditions
  • Manage your print subscription
  • Subscribe to Herald Premium
NZ Listener
  • NZ Listener e-edition
  • Contact Listener Editorial
  • Advertising with NZ Listener
  • Manage your Listener subscription
  • Subscribe to NZ Listener digital
  • Subscribe to NZ Listener
  • Subscriber FAQs
  • Subscription terms & conditions
  • Promotion and subscriber benefits
NZME Network
  • NZ Listener
  • The New Zealand Herald
  • The Northland Age
  • The Northern Advocate
  • Waikato Herald
  • Bay of Plenty Times
  • Rotorua Daily Post
  • Hawke's Bay Today
  • Whanganui Chronicle
  • Viva
  • Newstalk ZB
  • BusinessDesk
  • OneRoof
  • Driven Car Guide
  • iHeart Radio
  • Restaurant Hub
NZME
  • About NZME
  • NZME careers
  • Advertise with NZME
  • Digital self-service advertising
  • Book your classified ad
  • Photo sales
  • NZME Events
  • © Copyright 2025 NZME Publishing Limited
TOP